Discussion
Single-question measures of satisfaction can decrease patient burden and limit overlap with measures of communication effectiveness and perceived empathy. Both long and short questionnaires addressing satisfaction and perceived empathy show substantial ceiling effect. We compared 4 different measures for overall scores, floor and ceiling effect, and skewness and kurtosis, and assessed the correlation between scaled satisfaction and psychological status. We found that scale type influenced the median helpfulness score. As one would expect, scales with less ceiling effect have lower median scores. In other words, if the goal is to collect meaningful information and identify areas for improvement, there must be a willingness to accept lower scores.
Only the nonnumerical VAS was below the threshold of 15% ceiling effect proposed by Terwee et al.14 This scale with 3 anchor points and no visible numbers showed the least ceiling effect (12%) and minimal skew (–1.0), and was closer to kurtosis consistent with a normal distribution (5.0). However, the 11-point ordinal Likert scale with 5 anchor points and visible numbers had the lowest skewness and kurtosis (–0.58 and 4.0). The low ceiling effect observed with the nonnumerical VAS (12%) might be explained by the fact that the scale does not lead patients to a specific description of the helpfulness of their visit, but rather asks patients to use their own judgement in making the rating. The ordinal scale approached the most normal data distribution, and this might be explained by the presence of numbers on the scale. Ratings based on a 0-10 scale are commonly used, and familiarity with the system might have allowed people to pick a number that represents their actual view of the visit helpfulness, rather than picking the highest possible choice (which would have led to a ceiling effect). Study results comparing Likert scales and VAS are conflicting,15 with some preferring Likert scales for their responsiveness16 and ease of use in practice,17 and others preferring VAS for their sensitivity to describe continuous, subjective phenomenon and their high validity and reliability.18 Looking at our nonnumerical VAS, adding numbers to a scale might not help avoid, and may actually increase, the presence of ceiling effect. However, with the ordinal scale with visible numbers, we saw a 21% ceiling effect coupled with low skew and kurtosis (–0.58 and 4.0), which indicate that the distribution of scores is relatively normal. This finding is in line with other study results.19
Our findings demonstrated that feedback concerning self-efficacy, health anxiety, or depression had no or only a small effect on patient satisfaction. Consistent with prior evidence, psychological factors had limited or no correlation with satisfaction.20-24 Given the effect that priming has on patient-reported outcome measures, the effect of psychological factors on satisfaction could be an area of future study.
The NPS varied substantially based on scale structure. Increasing the spread of the scores to limit the ceiling effect will likely reduce promoters and detractors and increase neutrals. NPS systems have been used in the past to measure patient satisfaction with common hand surgery techniques and with community mental health services.25,26 These studies suggest that NPS could be a helpful addition to commonly used clinical measures of satisfaction, after more research has been done to validate it. The evidence showing that NPS are strongly influenced by scale structure suggests that NPS should be used and interpreted with caution.
Several caveats regarding this study should be kept in mind. This study specifically addressed ratings of visit helpfulness. Differently phrased questions might lead to different results. More work is needed to determine the essence of satisfaction with a medical visit.1 In addition, the majority of our patient population was white, employed, and privately insured, limiting generalizability to other populations with different demographics. Finally, all patients were seen by an orthopedic surgeon, and our results might not apply to other populations or clinical settings. However, given the scope of this study, we suspect that the findings can be generalized to specialty care in general and likely all medical contexts.